Items where City Author is "Yarrow, Kielan"
Article
Arnold, D. H., Clendinen, M., Johnston, A. (2024). The precision test of metacognitive sensitivity and confidence criteria. Consciousness and Cognition, 123, article number 103728. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2024.103728
Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163, Solomon, J. A. ORCID: 0000-0001-9976-4788, Arnold, D. H. (2023). The best fitting of three contemporary observer models reveals how participants’ strategy influences the window of subjective synchrony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 49(12), pp. 1534-1563. doi: 10.1037/xhp0001154
Saurels, B. W., Johnston, A., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2023). Event Probabilities Have a Different Impact on Early and Late Electroencephalographic Measures Regarded as Metrics of Prediction. The Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 36(1), pp. 187-199. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_02076
Silas, J., Jones, A., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2023). Spatial attention is not affected by alpha or beta transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation: a registered report. Cortex, 164(1), pp. 33-50. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.03.011
Suarels, B. W., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163, Lipp, O. V. (2023). The temporal visual oddball effect is not caused by repetition suppression. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 85(6), pp. 1755-1760. doi: 10.3758/s13414-023-02730-4
Arnold, D. H., Johnston, A., Adie, J. (2023). On why we lack confidence in some signal-detection-based analyses of confidence. Consciousness and Cognition, 113, article number 103532. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2023.103532
Saurels, B. W., Arnold, D. H., Anderson, N. L. (2022). The perceived duration of expected events depends on how the expectation is formed. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 84(5), pp. 1718-1725. doi: 10.3758/s13414-022-02519-x
Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163, Kohl, C., Segasby, T. (2022). Neural-latency noise places limits on human sensitivity to the timing of events. Cognition, 222, article number 105012. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.105012
Tapper, K. ORCID: 0000-0001-9097-6311, Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163, Farrar, S. T. (2022). Effects of calorie labelling and contextual factors on hypothetical coffee shop menu choices. Appetite, 172, article number 105963. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2022.105963
Hohaia, W., Saurels, B. W., Johnston, A. (2022). Occipital alpha-band brain waves when the eyes are closed are shaped by ongoing visual processes. Scientific Reports, 12(1), article number 1194. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05289-6
Saurels, B. W., Frommelt, T., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2022). Neural prediction errors depend on how an expectation was formed. Cortex, 147, pp. 102-111. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.10.012
Saurels, B. W., Hohaia, W., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2021). Visual predictions, neural oscillations and naïve physics. Scientific Reports, 11(1), article number 16127. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95295-x
Rowe, P. J., Haenschel,, C., Khachatoorian, N. (2020). Post-stroke object affordances: An EEG investigation. Brain and Cognition, 146, article number 105639. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105639
Kohl, C., Spieser, L., Forster, B. ORCID: 0000-0001-5126-7854 (2020). Centroparietal activity mirrors the decision variable when tracking biased and time-varying sensory evidence. Cognitive Psychology, 122, article number 101321. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2020.101321
Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163, Samba, C., Kohl, C. (2020). Auditory and Visual Durations Load a Unitary Working-Memory Resource. Timing & Time Perception, 9(1), pp. 1-38. doi: 10.1163/22134468-bja10013
Ioakeimidis, V., Haenschel, C. ORCID: 0000-0001-7855-2735, Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2020). A Meta-analysis of Structural and Functional Brain Abnormalities in Early-Onset Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin Open, 1(1), article number sgaa016. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa016
Farrar, S. T., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 & Tapper, K. ORCID: 0000-0001-9097-6311 (2020). The Effect of Mindfulness on Cognitive Reflection and Reasoning. Mindfulness, 11(9), pp. 2150-2160. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01429-z
Arnold, D. H., Hohaia, W. & Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2020). Neural correlates of subjective timing precision and confidence. Scientific Reports, 10(1), article number 3098. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59322-7
Saurels, B. W., Lipp, O. V., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2019). Predictable events elicit less visual and temporal information uptake in an oddball paradigm. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 82(3), pp. 1074-1087. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01899-x
Jalali, S., Martin, S. E., Ghose, T. (2019). Information accrual from the period preceding racket-ball contact for tennis ground strokes: Inferences from stochastic masking. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(AUG), article number 1969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01969
Kohl, C., Spieser, L., Forster, B. ORCID: 0000-0001-5126-7854 (2019). The Neurodynamic Decision Variable in Human Multi-Alternative Perceptual Choice. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(2), pp. 262-277. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01347
Gorst, T., Freeman, J., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2019). Assessing plantar sensation in the foot using the FOot Roughness Discrimination Test (FoRDT™): a reliability and validity study in stroke. PM&R, 11(10), pp. 1083-1092. doi: 10.1002/pmrj.12085
Mandeville, K. L., Barker, R., Packham, A. (2019). Financial interests of patient organisations contributing to technology appraisal at England's National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE): a policy review. BMJ, 364, article number k5300. doi: 10.1136/bmj.k5300
Gorst, T., Freeman, J., Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2019). Assessing lower limb position sense in stroke using the Gradient Discrimination Test (GradDT™) and Step-height Discrimination Test (StepDT™): a reliability and validity study. Disability and Rehabilitation, 42(15), pp. 2215-2223. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2018.1554008
Jalali, S., Martin, S. E., Murphy, C. P. (2018). Classification videos reveal the visual information driving complex real-world speeded decisions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(NOV), article number 2229. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02229
Hadar, A. A., Lazarovits, A. & Yarrow, K. ORCID: 0000-0003-0666-2163 (2018). Increased motor cortex excitability for concealed visual information. Journal of Psychophysiology, 33(4), pp. 286-295. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000230
Mandeville, K. L., Satherley, R-M., Hall, J. (2018). The political views of doctors in the United Kingdom: A cross sectional study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 72(10), pp. 880-887. doi: 10.1136/jech-2018-210801
Spieser, L., Kohl, C., Forster, B. ORCID: 0000-0001-5126-7854 (2018). Neurodynamic Evidence Supports a Forced-Excursion Model of Decision-Making under Speed/Accuracy Instructions. eNeuro, 5(3), ENEURO.0159-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0159-18.2018
Ball, D., Arnold, D. H. & Yarrow, K. (2017). Weighted integration suggests that visual and tactile signals provide independent estimates about duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(5), pp. 868-880. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000368
Rowe, P., Haenschel, C., Kosilo, M. (2017). Objects rapidly prime the motor system when located near the dominant hand. Brain & Cognition, 113, pp. 102-108. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.11.005
Yarrow, K. (2017). Assessing Risk Factors for Athletic Excellence. Progress in Brain Research, 232, pp. 141-144. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.11.008
Hadar, A. A., Rowe, P., Di Costa, S. (2016). Motor-evoked potentials reveal a motor-cortical readout of evidence accumulation for sensorimotor decisions. Psychophysiology, 53(11), pp. 1721-1731. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12737
Merchant, H. & Yarrow, K. (2016). How the motor system both encodes and influences our sense of time. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 8, pp. 22-27. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.01.006
Yarrow, K., Martin, S. E., Di Costa, S. (2016). A Roving Dual-Presentation Simultaneity-Judgment Task to Estimate the Point of Subjective Simultaneity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(MAR), article number 416. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00416
Keane, B., Spence, M., Yarrow, K. (2015). Perceptual confidence demonstrates trial-by-trial insight into the precision of audio-visual timing encoding. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, pp. 107-117. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.10.010
Shankleman, M., Sykes, C., Mandeville, K. L. (2015). Standardised (plain) cigarette packaging increases attention to both text-based and graphical health warnings: experimental evidence. Public Health, 129(1), pp. 37-42. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2014.10.019
Yarrow, K., Minaei, S. & Arnold, D. H. (2015). A model-based comparison of three theories of audiovisual temporal recalibration. Cognitive Psychology, 83, pp. 54-76. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.10.002
Tretyak, V. & Yarrow, K. (2014). Motor plans persist to influence subsequent actions with four or more response alternatives. Acta Psychologica, 149, pp. 9-17. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.013
Mandeville, K. L., O'Neill, S., Brighouse, A. (2014). Academics and competing interests in H1N1 influenza media reporting. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 68(3), pp. 197-203. doi: 10.1136/jech-2013-203128
Narkiewicz, M., Lambrechts, A., Eichelbaum, F. (2014). Humans don’t time sub-second intervals like a stopwatch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 41(1), pp. 249-263. doi: 10.1037/a0038284
Makris, S., Hadar, A. A. & Yarrow, K. (2013). Are object affordances fully automatic? A case of covert attention. Behavioral Neuroscience, 127(5), pp. 797-802. doi: 10.1037/a0033946
Makris, S., Grant, S., Hadar, A. A. (2013). Binocular vision enhances a rapidly evolving affordance priming effect: Behavioural and TMS evidence. Brain and Cognition, 83(3), pp. 279-287. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.004
Yarrow, K., Sverdrup-Stueland, I., Roseboom, W. (2013). Sensorimotor temporal recalibration within and across limbs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(6), pp. 1678-1689. doi: 10.1037/a0032534
Hadar, A. A., Makris, S. & Yarrow, K. (2012). The truth-telling motor cortex: Response competition in M1 discloses deceptive behaviour. Biological Psychology, 89(2), pp. 495-502. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.019
Arnold, D. H., Nancarrow, K. & Yarrow, K. (2012). The critical events for motor-sensory temporal recalibration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(AUGUST), article number 235. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00235
Yarrow, K., Roseboom, W. & Arnold, D. H. (2011). Spatial grouping resolves ambiguity to drive temporal recalibration.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), pp. 1657-1661. doi: 10.1037/a0024235
Arnold, D. H. & Yarrow, K. (2011). Temporal recalibration of vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1705), pp. 535-538. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1396
Yarrow, K., Jahn, N., Durant, S. (2011). Shifts of criteria or neural timing? The assumptions underlying timing perception studies. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), pp. 1518-1531. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.07.003
Yarrow, K., Brown, P. & Krakauer, J. W. (2009). Inside the brain of an elite athlete: The neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(8), pp. 585-596. doi: 10.1038/nrn2672
Ley, I., Haggard, P. & Yarrow, K. (2009). Optimal integration of auditory and vibrotactile information for judgments of temporal order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), pp. 1005-1019. doi: 10.1037/a0015021
Sauleau, P., Eusebio, A., Thevathasan, W. (2009). Involvement of the subthalamic nucleus in engagement with behaviourally relevant stimuli. European Journal Of Neuroscience, 29(5), pp. 931-942. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06635.x
Yarrow, K., Haggard, P. & Rothwell, J. C. (2008). Vibrotactile-auditory interactions are post-perceptual. Perception, 37(7), pp. 1114-1130. doi: 10.1068/p5824
Whiteley, L., Yarrow, K., Haggard, P. (2006). Biases in the perceived timing of perisaccadic perceptual and motor events. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 68(7), pp. 1217-1226. doi: 10.3758/bf03193722
Yarrow, K., Whiteley, L., Rothwell, J. C. (2006). Spatial consequences of bridging the saccadic gap. Vision Research, 46(4), pp. 545-555. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.04.019
Yarrow, K., Haggard, P. & Rothwell, J. C. (2004). Action, arousal and subjective time. Consciousness and Cognition, 13(2), pp. 373-390. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2003.10.006
Yarrow, K. & Rothwell, J. C. (2003). Manual chronostasis: Tactile perception precedes physical contact. Current Biology, 13(13), pp. 1134-1139. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00413-5
Yarrow, K., Haggard, P., Heal, R. (2001). Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity. Nature, 414(6861), pp. 302-305. doi: 10.1038/35104551
Yarrow, K., Brown, P., Gresty, M. A. (2001). Force platform recordings in the diagnosis of primary orthostatic tremor. Gait and posture, 13(1), pp. 27-34. doi: 10.1016/s0966-6362(00)00097-7
Book Section
Yarrow, K. & Obhi, S. S. (2014). Temporal perception in the context of action. In: Arstila, V. & Lloyd, D. (Eds.), Subjective Time; The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality. (pp. 455-476). The MIT Press.